Shevin Klein 1984

download Shevin Klein 1984

of 9

Transcript of Shevin Klein 1984

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    1/9

    Research & Practice for Persons with Severe Disabihties

    2004, Vol. 29, No. 3, 161-168

    copyright 2004 by

    T A S H

    CLASSIC TASH ARTICLE III

    Reprinted from   The Joumai of The Association for the Severely

     Handicapped

    Volume

     9(3),

     F all, 1984

    The Importance of Choice Making Skills

    For Students with Severe Disabilities

    Mayer Shevin and Nancy K. Klein.

    Author Information

    Mayer Sh evin, Ph .D ., Assistant Professor, Specialized

    Instructional Programs, Cleveland State University.

    Nancy K. Klein, Ph .D ., Professor, Specialized Instruc-

    tional Programs, Cleveland State University.

    Article Descriptors

    autonomy; behavior management; choice-making; curriculum

    design; independence; self-management; severely retarded

    Making choices is an essential part of functioning

    independently as an adult in society. Activities

    specifically directed toward helping students leam to

    make choices, however, are typically neglected in

    educational programs for students with severe

    disabilities. This paper presents a rationale for the in-

    clusion of choice-making as an imp ortant skill area for

    persons with severe disabilities. Behavioral approaches

    that present useful means of conceptualizing choice-

    making in the school life of severely disabled students

    are identified. Pro cedures are suggested for teaching

    choice-making skills, for integrating opportunities for

    exercising those skills throughou t the course of the stu-

    dent  s school life, and or fostering choice-making skills

    through the use of logical consequences.

    During the past decade, implementation of the nor-

    malization principle has resulted in a restructuring of

    educational and residential services for persons with

    severe handicaps. T his system change h as been accom -

    panied by a reexamination of curricular content for

    these students. Whereas severely handicapped persons

    in institutions and in schools traditionally have been

    taught compliance, more recent training programs

    reflect new expectations and views of people with

    severe disabilities. Professionals are now focusing on

    strategies to enable these citizens to avail themselves

    of the opportunities inherent in community facilities

    by transferring the focus from compliance to foster-

    ing independence and self-reliance (Wehman &

    McLaughlin, 1981).

    One aspect of teaching independence that has been

    neglected is teaching severely handicapped persons to

    make choices. T his paper presents a rationale for the

    need to structure teaching activities to enable these

    students with severe handicaps to exercise their own

    choice in school, at home, and in community settings.

    Some possible areas for classroom activities are sug-

    gested, including (a) the design and im plementation of

    age-appropriate experiences; and (b) the creation of

    opportunities, both in and out of school, for exercis-

    ing choice.

    For non-handicapped children, little or no formal at-

    tention is paid in school to the establishment of skills

    related to autonomy and to choice-making; however,

    most children will nevertheless acquire such skills dur-

    ing their childhood. In contrast, we cannot predict

    with any confidence that these autonomous skills will

    develop in this informal fashion for students who are

    retarded. Retarded students are likely to have very

    limited opportunity for learning through interaction

    with, and observation of, normal agemates and older

    children (Porter, Ramsey, Tremblay, laccobo, &

    Crawley, 1978). Such self-reliance is important to the

    quality of life of individuals who are retarded if they

    are to reap the benefits of norm alized services. Schools

    must take responsibility for teaching such skills directly

    and explicitly. Concurren tly, analysis of current pro-

    grams must be undertaken to determine which aspects

    of programm ing serve to deter the development of self-

    reliance and independence related to making choices.

    Students who are severely retarded may require ex-

    tensive assistance in developing the skills necessary for

    exercising choice, rather tJian merely following

    teachers' or parents' leads. Learning to make choices

    can extend to areas of major life concerns such as voca-

    tion, residence, and consent to medical treatment, as

    well as to relatively minor, but cumulatively signifi-

    cant day-to-day decisions such as choice of recreational

    activities, partners for social activities, food, clo thing,

    music, seating, and scheduling.

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    2/9

    162

    Shevin and Klein

    Defining and Assessing Choice-Making

    Deflning choice making

    Most studies which look at choice-making in peo-

    ple who are severely retarded (Holvoet, Brewer.

    Mulligan, Guess, Helmstetter, & Riggs, 1981; Mithaug

    & Hanawalt, 1978; Mithaug & Mar, 1980; Wehman,

    1979;

      and Wuerch & Voeltz, 1982) do not include

    definitions of cho ice-m akin g

    per se.

     There does not

    appear to be a common definition of choice-making

    in this field that addresses the student's desires as in-

    tegral to the process. In other words, helping the child

    to understand what she actually wants is not usually

    considered as part of th act of' 'choosing something.' '

    The absence of such a feature in a working definition

    of ch o ic e prevents professionals in the field from

    focusing on issues related to the disabled individual's

    role in setting her own objectives, and in exercising

    autonomous control as a consumer of habilitative and

    educational services. W ithout a view of choice-making

    that includes teaching children to identify personal

    preferences, professionals and advocates are at risk of

    mistaking lack of protest for informed consent, habitual

    behavior for active choice, and resignation to one's lot

    for contentment with that lot. There is a need to be

    able to determine, in a given situation, whether or not

    a student or client is exercising real autonomy.

    This determination is most difficult for those with

    seriously limited behavioral repertoires. Two examples

    will illustrate the complexity of this issue:

    1.

      A teacher wishes to teach a student to operate a

    soft-drink machine, so that she may purchase a drink

    during breaktime. The student, in her current opera-

    tion of the machine, puts the coins in the coin slot, and

    then, without directing her eyes at the machine, slaps

    her hand against the machine's face until she hits one

    of the panels. W hen a can of soda falls into the recep-

    tacle, she picks it up, Of)ens it and drinks the soda. Can

    that student be said to have  chosen  that particular

    brand?

    2. A w orkshop supervisor asks a worker who is sit-

    ting at a table containing the materials for an assembly

    task to begin screwing the nut-and-bolt sets together.

    Two minutes later, the supervisor sees that no sets have

    been assembled by that worker. Is the supervisor to

    consider the worker as having been non-compliant, as

    having misunderstood the task, or as having chosen

    not to participate?

    For the purposes of this paper, ch oo sin g is de-

    fined as  the act ofan individual's selection of a pre-

    ferred alternative from amo ng several familiar options.

    The use of the word pr ef er re d in this definition

    highlights the assumption that people with severe

    disabilities have personal preferences, likes and

    dislikes, which they can learn to identify for themselves

    through ongoing educational experiences. It is assumed

    that persons with severe handicaps can learn to express

    their preferences, and that parents, teachers and

    managers in their environment can learn to be respon-

    sive to both typical and atypical expression s of choice.

    The emphasis on selection among familiar options

    highlights the longitudinal nature of education and pro-

    gramming for meaningful choice-making.

    Research in choice making

    For the special educator engaged in fostering choice-

    making activities in students who are severely retarded,

    two areas of the research literature are of particular

    interest: (1) research that documents the effects of

    choice-making activities on learning, classroom

    behavior, skill generalization, and independent func-

    tioning in regular and special education settings; and

    (2) studies that address the specific problems related

    to assessing and developing choice-making skills in

    students who are severely retarded.

    Holvoet, Brewer, Mulligan, Guess, Helmstetter and

    Riggs (1981 ), in a survey of the experimental literature

    related to the effect of choices on learning rate and stu-

    dent motivation, found few studies in which the effects

    of choice-making on school behavior had been studied

    in students who are retarded. However, they identified

    several studies from the regular education literature

    (Berk, 1976; Felixbrod & O'L eary , 1973; Hock stra,

    1978; Wh ite, 1977) which docum ented the positive ef-

    fects of choice-making on learning rate. Researchers

    who have looked at choice-making by individuals who

    are severely retarded (Holvoet et al., 1981; Mithaug

    & Hanawalt, 1978; Mithaug & Mar, 1980) have found

    that one cannot assume that a student has made a choice

    simply because that student has acted in a situation in

    which choice is permitted. Mithaug and Hanawalt

    (1978) have described the ambiguities which arise

    when individuals who are severely retarded are

    presented with opportunities for choice-making, since

    these individuals may lack precise verbal responses,

    facial exp ressions, task avoidance behav iors, and other

    means by whi ch pre fe rences a re t yp i ca l l y

    communicated.

    In studies with students who are severely retarded,

    as in studies with no nhandicapped students, choice ap-

    pears to be seen as an y student behav ior, deemed ap-

    propriate by the experimenter, in a setting in which

    more than one behavior is deemed appro pria te, pro-

    vided only that the experimenter has demonstrated that

    the student's behavior relative to the various possible

    tasks is somewhat consistent over time. In other w ords,

    our profession has focused on choice-making as a per-

    missible activity, rather than as a teaching target. For

    many students who are severely retarded, this may

    mean that they remain dependent on others for direc-

    tion, even though the theoretical possibility of choice

    does exist. In contrast, this paper suggests that choice-

    making is a viable teaching target, to be subjected to

    task analysis, planning, implementation and evaluation

    similar to those which are characteristic of more tradi-

    tional content areas.

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    3/9

    Choice-Making

    163

    Assessing choice making

    Some of the information needed to determine

    whether choosing is taking place on any given occa-

    sion can be identified through the use of the A B C

    analytic model of applied behavior analysis (e.g.

    Sulzer-Azaroff & Mayer, 1977). The use of this model

    is enhanced when observers incorporate additional

    behavioral data that are not typically collected or valued

    within the applied behavior analytic model. This in-

    cludes information related to the student's previous

    history, which m ay not be easily available to the clini-

    cian or teacher. Also of importance are descriptions

    of behaviors that are not previously specified in

    research designs or IEPs as target behav iors . Due

    to the precision of descriptive observation required,

    and the inability of professionals to predict in advance

    the sponta neou s beh avi ors being looked for, such

    behaviors do not easily lend themselves to quantitative

    recording or reductionist coding procedures.

    Rather than viewing cho ice-making as a single target

    behavior, an array of related behaviors that comprise

    the target must be considered. For example, in describ-

    ing choice-making related to workshop tasks, such an

    array might include quantitative data on latency, rate

    and intensity of on-task behavior, descriptive informa-

    tion on socialization, facial expression and body

    language during the task, and responses to interview

    questions related to the workshop tasks among which

    choices are to be made.

    In order to arrive at a meaningful characterization

    of choice-making, educators need to go beyond the

    typical scope of applied behavior analysis. Fortunate-

    ly, there are alternative behavioral approaches, each

    with a substantial theoretical and research base, upon

    which professionals can draw:

    1. Studies of verbal interaction: Although most of

    their work has centered on issues of affective and social

    development. Bell and Ainsworth (1972) have iden-

    tified in operational terms the concept of the

      respon-

    sive parent,

      and have documen ted the role of that adult

    in infant development. In their study, the important

    variables in parental behavior that were related to the

    social and affective development of their children were

    related to the degree to which the parents were respon-

    sive, that is, the degree to which they came under the

    control of the expressive behavior (i.e ., the crying) of

    their children. (For a review of this literature in rela-

    tion to crying, see Shevin, 1979.) Recent work by

    Rogers-Warren (1981) has documented the effects of

    such parent-child interaction on more advanced levels

    of language development, and has provided a basis for

    characterizing such interactions behaviorally. Work by

    Sapon and Kaczmarek (1975) and Kaczmarek (1977)

    in the descriptive analysis of verbal behavior provides

    us with an analytic framework for looking at behavioral

    development as an ongoing expansion of correspond-

    ing productive and receptive verbal repertoires between

    children and those around them. These models are ap-

    plicable in the study of choice-making, in that they pro-

    vide the basis for a global characterization of adult-

    child relationships in terms of the degree to which the

    adult's behavior is guided by that of the child, as well

    as providing a means of studying the shifts which take

    place in those adult-child relationships over time.

    2.  Environmentat psychoiogy and ecological

    behaviorism:

     In their analysis of the practice of body-

    rocking in residential institutions for retarded persons,

    Klaber and Butterfield (1968) studied body-rock ing not

    as an indication of the deviance or deficits of the in-

    dividuals displaying the behavior, but rather as a

    diagnostic indicator of the level of care and the ap-

    propriateness of program ming received by the residents

    in various institutions. Rogers-Warren and Warren

    (1977), in a collection entitled

      Ecological Perspec-

    tives in Behavior Ana lysis,

      ha ve assembled the works

    of authors who provide models for looking at behavior

    not as a series of isolated target behaviors, but as net-

    works of interactive behaviors existing in a material

    and social context, in which students and teachers are

    both interactive participants.

    These broader perspect ives and contextual

    frameworks provide richer descriptions of those

    behaviors which, when viewed narrowly, are often

    characterized as off-ta sk, non-co mp liant or de -

    via nt behavio rs. For the student with limited social

    and communicative repertoires, it may be precisely

    those off-task behaviors which represent the most ef-

    fective current m eans of exercising some control ov er

    the environment, and are thus the best available ex-

    pression of personal choice.

    A number of common features are found in the ap-

    proaches described above, which are pertinent to the

    area of choice-making. They provide special contexts

    for considering students' behavior:

    1. professional attention is focused on the similarities

    which exist among all people, rather than on deficits

    and differences;

    2.

      descriptions of behavior take into account the

    transactional nature of human interactions, consider-

    ing the activities of all of the participants in an interac -

    tion, not only the actions of the deviant one; and

    3.  an attempt to describe students' behavior, to the

    greatest extent possible, from the students' perspec-

    tive, rather than from those of the teacher or the in-

    stitution, undergirds this process.

    Notes Toward a

    Choice-Fostering Curriculum

    Professionals concerned with fostering choice-

    making in students with severe disabilities are travel-

    ing in relatively uncharted terrritory, without the com-

    forts of traditional behavioral definitions and research

    methodologies. However, the lack of a precise,

    familiar terminology should not deter professionals

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    4/9

    164

    Shevin and Klein

    from taking steps toward fostering those skills in

    whatever settings they are working. A multiplicity of

    approaches by professionals and advocates sharing a

    common commitment to individual rights and in-

    dividual development will provide the experiential basis

    out of which comparative research methodologies and

    new evaluation designs can grow.

    As instructional procedures and behavioral targets

    related to choice-making are developed, two reference

    points can guide these efforts: identification of ways

    in which most citizens typically indicate and exercise

    choice in our society, and identification of mechanisms,

    however atypical, by which students with severe

    disabilities currently make their wishes known. The

    goal, of course, is to teach students culturally nor-

    mative ways of exercising choice, as alternatives to

    biting, flinging or screaming. However, teachers must

    avoid being trapped into the absurdity of spending

    many years prodding a non-compliant child through

    a choice-making readiness pro gra m, while label-

    ing the behaviors by which the child may be currently

    demonstrating preferences as stubborn or ma nipulative.

    Successful fostering of choice-making will require

    both systematic teaching of new skills, and provision

    of opportunities to practice those skills in the classroom

    and in natural settings at home and in the community.

    There are at least three essential components to a

    choice-fostering curriculum: (a) cognitive/discrimina-

    tion skill clusters; (b) affective skill clusters; and (c)

    generalization of skills in real-life experiences.

    Included in the

     cognitive/discrimination skill clusters

    are those skills which enable the learner to understand

    and discriminate from among alternatives as a prere-

    quisite to acting. Among the skills in this component

    are visual, auditory, tactile and gustatory discrimina-

    tion skills, and the understanding of such concepts as

      c h o o s e , f i n i sh , n o w , l a t e r , m o r e , a nd

    so forth.

    The

     affective skill cluster

      includes concepts such as

      I l ike/don't l ik e, I prefe r, I want to/don' t want

    to d o , and so forth. These affective terms are in-

    troduced in a choice-making episode at the time that

    the choice is actually made.

    The

      real-life experiences

      are the activities, both in-

    side and outside of the classroom, which form the con-

    tent of choices to be made. These include choices of:

    food, clothing, toys, activities, activity partners, etc.

    Classroom choices may also include choices available

    within behavior contracting systems, such as choices

    of reinforcers, target behaviors, working conditions,

    etc.

    The age and co mpetenc e of the child are critical fac-

    tors to consider when selecting real-life experiences

    for the classroom. Therefore, the teacher must assess

    the significance and/or risk of the choices presented

    in relation to the age and ability of the child. For ex-

    ample, a four-year-old child might have the opportunity

    to choose among various snacks and drinks at snack-

    time, but would not be allowed to choose between

    wearing a lightweight jacket or a snowsuit when the

    outside temperature is below freezing. As the child

    becomes older and, with increasing practice, more

    adept at choice-making, the significance, that is the

    consequences of that child's choices, must grow ac-

    cordingly. For example, a twenty-year-old who has

    learned to m ake choices might be given the opportunity

    to choose an apartment. This real-life choice, while

    beyond the capability of a four-year-old child, is also

    beyond the capability of the student labeled retarded

    who has had no previous experience with choice-

    making. As retarded children move toward adulthood,

    they will also benefit from choice-making which ex-

    pands from choices in a small number of areas to

    choices across a broad number of domains.

    Brown et al. (1980) have identified a process for the

    establishment of appropriate educational plans for

    students with severe disabilities. These procedures in-

    clude strategies for inventories involving students, their

    parents and guardians, and the community en-

    vironments within which the students currently or

    potentially function. Such individualized and

    ecologically based strategies for identification of cur-

    ricular content and instructional design are directly ap-

    plicable to the determination of the highest priority

    areas for the targeting of choice-making activities. The

    single departure from the model described by Brown

    et al. (1980) is that, in their list of 16 dim ension s to

    be considered in prioritizing curricular content, stu-

    dent preference is listed as one item, without assum-

    ing any special importance. For obvious reasons, in

    the curricular area of choice-fostering skills, student

    preference will, by definition, assume a position of

    highest consideration in prioritizing goals.

    Thre e contexts for fostering choice-m aking skills in

    severely retarded students will be described below.

    These include:

    1. classroom activities designed to teach specific

    choice-making skills;

    2.  integration of choice-making opportunities

    throughout the student's day, across curricular

    domains; and

    3.  provision of opportun ities, both inside and out-

    side of school, for students to experience the

    benefits and consequences of choices they have

    made.

    Fostering Choices Through Specific Curricular

    Units

    The following procedures are suggested as an ex-

    ample of the activities that a hypothetical teacher of

    four- to six-year-old children with severe disabilities

    might follow. They will serve to illustrate the inclu-

    sion of specific choice-fostering content in a school's

    curriculum.

    1. Content selection — the ages and abilities of the

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    5/9

    Choice-Making

    165

    children in the class are considered prior to selecting

    a real-life experien ce on w hich to focus. LFsing the pro -

    cedures described by Brown et al. (1980) for the

    establishment of functional, age-appropriate objectives,

    a teacher of young children may select food as an ap-

    propriate category for teaching choice-making skills.

    2 .  Cognitive/discrimination skill cluster

     — iden-

    tification of the discrimination skills used when a child

    makes food choices . These skills include some m ethod

    of indicating choice (e.g., pointing), and the ability to

    discriminate between foods of similar appearance ( e.g .,

    oranges and lemons). Following the visual discrimina-

    tion activities, the child is given the opportunity to ex-

    perience the differences in taste of these objects. Such

    visual and gustatory discrimination skills will form the

    foundation for subsequent affectively based decision-

    making.

    3.  Affective skill cluster  — once the child has

    discriminated among different foods, and has indicated

    through either typical or atypical communication modes

    which of these foods he prefers, the teacher models

    words or gestures that can be used to label the child's

    responses to the foods. As the child tastes, and subse-

    quently reaches for, the orange, the teacher may use

    words such as tastes go od , I like it , or tastes

    sw ee t. As the child tastes the lemon , and subsequently

    grimaces, refiises it or pushes it away, the teacher may

    use words such as tastes so ur, that 's not go od ,

    or I do n't like tha t. Initially, the teacher might use

    one food which is likely to be selected, and another

    which is likely to be rejected (e.g., apple and onion).

    Subsequently, the child can be presented with two

    desirable foods, and be taught to indicate a favorite.

    Consultation with parents is important at this stage. It

    is important for the teacher to be sensitive to expres-

    sions of student preference, to model the affective ter-

    minology, to point out to the child that a choice has

    been made, and to elicit feedback from the child about

    whether she likes the choice.

    Once these initial discriminations are mastered, and

    the child can systematically label preferences, the

    child's choice-making skills may be refined in the

    following ways:

    1.

      by decreasing the gradations of the discrimina-

    tions made within a domain (e.g., gross to

    fine);

    2 .

      by increasing the number of objects among

    which discriminations are made; and

    3.  by adding new dom ains within which

    discriminations are made.

    These examples assume that the students display

    relatively well developed receptive and expressive

    verbal repertoires. Additional concerns must be ad-

    dressed by teachers who work with students who have

    extremely limited communicative skills. For students

    who display non-standard or inconsistent communica-

    tive behavior, the teacher must frequently take pains

    to ascertain that the student's message has been ac-

    curately received. Duncan, Sbardellati, Maheady and

    Sainato (1981) have suggested a continuum of response

    modes to be used in testng and assessment situations,

    which lend themselves to the determination of student

    choices. They recommend that, in decreasing order of

    desirability, the following communication modes be

    used: (a) use of expressive language responses (vocal);

    (b) use of pointing respo nses; (c) use of existing m otor

    responses; (d) training of existing inconsistent

    responses; and (e) teaching of a nonexistent response

    (p .  18).

    One potential difficulty to which teachers must be

    attuned is that gestures and facial expressions, which

    have a commonly interpreted meaning in society at

    large, may be used inadvertently in non-standard ways

    by students. For example, a cJiild with limited com-

    munication skills or with cerebral palsy may smile

    when asked if she wants something to drink; the smile

    may  no t  necessarily indicate a y e s answer to the

    question.

    A second difficulty is that, for a student w hose

    limited commu nication includes a consistent y es /n o

    response, the student's preference in a given case may

    not be able to be expressed by a simple yes or no. For

    examp le: a teacher says to Albert, H er e's the

    xylophone, the trumpet, and the ocarina. Would you

    like to play with the tru m pe t? Albert nods his head

      y e s .

    The teacher hands Albert the trumpet. H e tums

    it over in his hands, looks into the mouthpiece, puts

    it down on the table, points to the ocarina, and wh ines.

    The teacher says, N o , Albert, you already chose the

    t rumpet .

    In this situation, Albert's preference may have been

      Let me look at the trumpet for a moment, and then

    I'll decide which instrument I'd like . How ever, when

    limited in his productive verbal behavior to a yes/no

    response, the closest he is able to come is to indicate

      y e s . The teacher, however, may erroneously con-

    clude only that Albert do esn't know what he wan ts.

    Such conflicts and misunderstandings are less likely

    to arise in situations in which the choices available to

    students are more open-ended, less limited ones, and

    in situations in which the student has many oppor-

    tunities to experience directly the various alternatives

    prior to making a choice. Fu rthermo re, ongoing com-

    munication amon g pare nts, teachers and other profes-

    sionals and caretakers in the student's environment,

    using formal systems such as the Communication

    Record (Kollinzas, 1983) or less formal anecdotal com-

    munications, will allow those directly involved with

    an individual to become aware of the student's

    preferences.

    Integrating Choice Making Throughout the School

    Day

    As important as the establishment of discrete choice-

    making skills might be , it will be of little avail if choice-

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    6/9

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    7/9

    Choice-Making

    167

    this child have been permitted to order the food that

    he probably dislikes, or should the teacher have in-

    terceded to prevent his having to face a distasteful

    lunch?

    In such a situation, it is important that the child be

    permitted to exercise his own initiative. To facilitate

    this,

     structured activities which involve tasting a variety

    of foods might be arranged to precede the child's ex-

    ercising choice in this situation. By allowing the child

    to live with his choice, the teacher provides the stu-

    dent with an excellent opportunity to understand the

    consequences of personal decision-making. Students

    who have been labeled as retarded must learn that for

    them, as for everyone else, som e consequences of their

    choices will be negative ones.

    Some teachers or parents seem to believe that we

    must not allow retarded individuals to fail. It seems

    unkind to withhold intervention when a retarded per-

    son is going to err. Ho wev er, persons who are retarded

    must not be denied the opportunity to learn from their

    own mistak es. Veach (1977) has forcefully described

    the importance of this type of experience for young

    children in general; Perske (1972) has made an ex-

    cellent case for th e dignity of ri sk as a critical ele-

    ment of normalization for those persons identified as

    retarded.

    Conclusion

    For y ears, special education p ractitioners h ave taken

    pride in their skill in identifying curricular areas of im-

    portance to students, defining these areas in ways

    which lend themselves to meaningful assessment and

    task analysis, and developing effective p rocedures for

    establishing skills by assisting students through small,

    incremental steps toward mastery. Within that process,

    we have sometimes lost track of a primary objective

    of special education — that is, the preparation of

    students wh o can function effectively  without our ongo-

    ing expert assistance. Ultimately, our success in

    meeting that objective must serve as the basis for

    evaluating how successful we have been in all the other

    objectives which we set for ourselves as teachers of,

    and advocates for, people with severe disabilities.

    References

    Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying

    and maternal responsiveness.  Child Development, 43,

    1171-1190.

    Berk, R. A. (1976). Effects of choice of instructional methods

    on verbal learning tasks.  Psychological Repo rts, 3 8,

    8 6 7 -8 7 0 .

    Brown, L., Falvey, M., Vincent, L., Kaye, N., Johnson,

    F .,   Ferrara-Parrish, P. , & Gruenewald, L. (1980).

    Strategies for generating comprehensive, longitudinal and

    chronological age appropriate individual educational plans

    for adolescent and young adult severely handicapped

    students. In L. Brown, M . Falvey, D . Baumgart, I . Pum-

    pian, J. Schroeder, & L. Gruenewald (Eds.) ,  Strategies

    for teaching chronological age appropriate functional skills

    to adolescent an d young adult severely handicapped

    students.  Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin -

    Madison, and Madison Metropolitan School District.

    Duncan, D., Sbardellati, E., Maheady, L., & Sainato, D.

    (1981). Nondiscriminatory assessment of severely

    physically handicapped individuals. Joumai of the Associa-

    tion for the Severely Han dicapped,  (5(2), 17-22.

    Felixbrod, J. J., & O'Leary, K. D. (1973). Effects of rein-

    forcement on children's academic behavior as a function

    of self-determined and externally imposed contingencies.

    Journal of Applied Beh avior Analysis, 6,  241 -25 0 .

    Guess, D ., & Siegel-Causey, E. (in press). Behavioral con-

    trol and education of severely handicapped students: Who's

    doing what to whom? and why? In D. Bricker & J. Filler

    (Eds. ) ,  Serving the severely retarded: From research to

    practice.  Reston, VA: The Council for Exceptional

    Children.

    Hockstra, C. M. (1978).

      The effects of choice of conse-

    quences and procedures on preschool children's rate of

    working arithmetic problems.  Unpublished doctoral disser-

    tation, Washington State University.

    Holvoet, J. , Brewer, M., Mulligan, M., Guess, D.,

    Helmstetter, E., & Riggs, P. (1981).  Influence of activity

    choice on learning among adolescent severely, multiply

    handicapped students.  Unpublished manuscript. Univer-

    sity of Kansas.

    Kaczmarek, L. A. (1977). A descriptive analysis of the home

    verbal environments of non-talking children.  Unpublished

    doctoral dissertation. University of Rochester.

    Klaber, M. M., & Butterfield, E. C. (1968). Stereotyped

    rocking

     —

     A measure of institution and ward effectiveness.

    American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73,  13-20.

    Kollinzas, G. (1983). The Communication Record: Sharing

    information to promote sign language generalization.  Jour-

    nal of the Association for the Severely Hand icapped,  S(3),

    4 9 - 5 5 .

    Mithaug, D. E., & Hanawalt, D. A. (1978). The validation

    of procedures lo assess prevocational task preferences in

    retarded adults.  Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,

    11(1),  153-162.

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    8/9

    168

    Shevin and Klein

    Mithaug, D. E. ,& M ar , D . K. (1980).

     The relation between

    choosing and working prevocational tasks  in two severe-

    ly retarded young adults.

      Journal of Applied Behavior

    Analysis,  J3{\),

      177-182.

    Perske, R. (1972). The dignity of risk.

     In W. Wolfensberger

    (Ed.),

      The principle of norm alization in huma n services.

    Toronto: National Institute on Mental

      Retardation.

    Porter, R. H., Ramsey, B., Tremblay, A., laccobo, M., &

    Crawley, S. (1978). Social interactions

      in heterogeneous

    groups of retarded and normally developing children: An

    observational study. In G. P. Sackett (Ed.),

      Observing

    behavior.

      V ol. 1. Baltim ore: University

      Park

      Press .

    Rogers-Warren, A. K.  (1981,  May). //

      takes two: Toward

    a behavioral analysis of mother-child interactions.

      Paper

    presented at the annual meeting of the Association for

    Behavior Analysis, Milwaukee, WI.

    Rogers-Warren, A.. & Warren, S. F. (Eds.) (1977).

    Ecological perspectives in behavior analysis.

      Baltimore,

    MD: University Park Press.

    Sapon, S. M., & Kaczmarek, L. A. (1975).

      The descriptive

    analysis of verbal behavior.

      Rochester, NY: Publications

    of the Verbal Behavior Laboratory.

    Shevin, M. (1979, June).

      Crying as productive verbal

    behavior.  Paper

      presented at the annual meeting of the

    Association for Behavior Analysis, Dearborn, MI.

    Sulzer-Azaroff

    B., & Mayer, G. R. (1977).

      Applying

    behavior analysis procedures with children and

     youth.  Ne w

    York:

      Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Veach, D. M. (1977). Choice with responsibility.

      Young

    Children, 32(4),

      2 2 - 2 5 .

    Wehm an, P . (1979). Teaching recreational skills to severe-

    ly and profoundly handicapped persons. In R. L. York

    & E. Edgar, (Eds.) ,

      Teaching the severely handicapp ed,

    Volume 4. Seattle, WA: American Association for the

    Education of the Severely/Profoundly Handicapped.

    Wehman, P. & McLaughlin, P. (1981).

      Program develop-

    ment in special education.

      New York: McGraw-Hill .

    White, A. M. (1977). The effects of choice upon paired-

    associate learning and reading comprehension in children.

    1974 undergraduate honors thesis, Virginia Polytechnic

    Institute and State University. As cited in Perlmuter. L.

    C , & Monty, R. A. The importance of perceived con-

    trol: Fact or fantasy.

      American Scientist, 65,

      7 5 9 -7 6 5 .

    Wuerch, B. B., & Voeltz, L. M. (1982).

     Longitudinal leisure

    skills for s everely handicap ped learners: The Ho 'onanea

    curriculum component.

      Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

  • 8/18/2019 Shevin Klein 1984

    9/9